Discussion
Deficient HL is prevalent in the USA, affecting nearly 40% of the adult population.11 In the current study, it is notable that we identified deficient HL in 40% of our study patients. Patients with deficient HL often have a limited knowledge of self-management skills and greater difficulty understanding prescription drug labels, contributing to high rates of non-adherence to treatment, emergency department visits, and hospital readmissions.11–16
The relationship between HL proficiency and postdischarge QOL among patients hospitalized for injury or emergency surgery has not been extensively evaluated. Prior studies of the relationship between QOL and HL have demonstrated inconsistent results.17 This may be attributable to relatively small sample sizes (usually a few hundred patients) and use of different instruments to assess both HL and QOL. In the current study, we have observed a weak association between HL proficiency and emotional well-being after hospital discharge. This is a somewhat intuitive finding; one might expect the HL-deficient patient to suffer some level of frustration after a healthcare-related emergency, be it an injury or an acute surgical disease. Nonetheless, there are obviously multiple contributors to emotional well-being beyond literacy that are beyond the scope of this study but are worthy of investigation.
In prior work, we identified that HL-deficient patients may perceive physician–patient communication during hospitalization less satisfactory than HL-proficient patients.6 In the current study, we did not replicate those findings. This may be attributable to the smaller sample size of the current study and the use of a different instrument to assess HL. We did, however, observe that a positive perception of physician compassion among patients with deficient HL contributed positively to postdischarge emotional well-being. Although the correlation between perception of physician compassion and emotional well-being is relatively weak, it nonetheless presents a target of opportunity to improve the postdischarge outcomes of our HL-deficient patients.
It is important to emphasize that what was evaluated was the perception of physician compassion rather than whether a physician was, in fact, compassionate. We think it is a reasonable assumption that most if not all our physicians provide compassionate care. However, it is prudent for those of us who provide patient care to reflect on whether our patients perceive our care as compassionate and identify personal areas for improvement, such as taking the time to sit, rather than stand at the bedside, which is a simple maneuver that is associated with improved patient perception of physician communication.18
There are a number of limitations of this study that are worth of highlighting. For one, we applied a trauma-specific QOL instrument to a mixed cohort of trauma and emergency surgery patients. Although the RT-QOL was not validated in an emergency general surgery patient population, these patients share certain attributes with injured patients and it is presumable that the RT-QOL would be applicable to this group. Specifically, these patients are similar to injured patients in that their illness is acute and unexpected and they undergo procedures that are urgent and usually without opportunity for deliberation. Second, it is impossible to determine which physicians were considered in a patient’s decision to score their perception of physician–patient communication given that they interact with both residents and attendings, and multiple specialties may be involved in the patient’s care. It is quite possible that a particularly negative interaction with one particular physician may have had an outsized influence on the patient’s perception of their experience. Third, the results of this study may not be generalizable. This study was performed in a single institution in the Southwestern United States, and it remains unclear if similar patient cohorts in other areas of the country would have similar outcomes, particularly with respect to RT-QOL, which was validated in a Northeastern patient population.8 Lastly, this study suffered a considerable loss of follow-up leaving open the possibility that the results may have differed significantly with a higher capture rate of those who initially agreed to participate but then did not respond when called to complete the RT-QOL.
In conclusion, we observed a weak correlation between HL proficiency and emotional well-being as measured by the RT-QOL instrument. In addition, we observed that a positive perception of physician compassion may contribute to emotional well-being soon after hospital discharge among HL-deficient patients. Improving patient perception of caregiver compassion during hospitalization may be a target of opportunity with respect to improving QOL after hospital discharge.